-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 16
/
Copy pathallpubs.bib
860 lines (758 loc) · 47.6 KB
/
allpubs.bib
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
@unpublished{vasishth2021sample,
title={Sample size determination for {Bayesian} hierarchical models commonly used in psycholinguistics},
author={Vasishth, Shravan and Yadav, Himanshu and Schad, Daniel and Nicenboim, Bruno},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/u8yvc},
year={2021},
eprint = {https://psyarxiv.com/u8yvc/},
publisher={PsyArXiv}
}
@unpublished{schad2021workflow,
title={Workflow Techniques for the Robust Use of Bayes Factors},
author={Daniel J. Schad and Bruno Nicenboim and Paul-Christian Bürkner and Michael Betancourt and Shravan Vasishth},
year={2021},
eprint={https://arxiv.org/abs/2103.08744},
archivePrefix={arXiv},
primaryClass={stat.ME},
notes={unpublished}
}
@article{Brochhagen2021,
doi = {10.1007/s42113-021-00099-x},
year = 2021,
publisher = {Springer Science and Business Media {LLC}},
author = {Thomas Brochhagen},
title = {Brief at the Risk of Being Misunderstood: Consolidating Population- and Individual-Level Tendencies},
journal = {Computational Brain {\&} Behavior},
customb = {https://osf.io/7m9np/}
}
@article{Spektor2018,
author = {Spektor, Mikhail Sergeevic and Kellen, David},
doi = {10.3758/s13423-018-1446-5},
journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin {\&} Review},
number = {6},
pages = {2047--2068},
title = {{The relative merit of empirical priors in non-identifiable and sloppy models: Applications to models of learning and decision-making}},
volume = {25},
year = {2018},
customb = {https://osf.io/2ws78/}
}
@article{Spektor2019,
author = {Spektor, Mikhail Sergeevic and Gluth, Sebastian and Fontanesi, Laura and Rieskamp, J{\"{o}}rg},
doi = {10.1037/rev0000122},
journal = {Psychological Review},
number = {1},
pages = {52--88},
title = {{How similarity between choice options affects decisions from experience: The accentuation-of-differences model}},
volume = {126},
year = {2019},
customb = {https://osf.io/w376r/}
}
@article{Fontanesi2019,
author = {Fontanesi, Laura and Gluth, Sebastian and Spektor, Mikhail Sergeevic and Rieskamp, J{\"{o}}rg},
doi = {10.3758/s13423-018-1554-2},
journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin {\&} Review},
number = {4},
pages = {1099--1121},
title = {{A reinforcement learning diffusion decision model for value-based decisions}},
volume = {26},
year = {2019},
customb = {https://osf.io/95d4p/}
}
@article{Kraemer2020,
author = {Kraemer, Peter Maximilian and Fontanesi, Laura and Spektor, Mikhail Sergeevic and Gluth, Sebastian},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01794-9},
journal = {Psychonomic Bulletin {\&} Review},
keywords = {Bayesian modeling,DDM,cognitive bias,decision making,diffusion model,dual-process,episodic memory,preferential choice},
title = {{Response time models separate single- and dual-process accounts of memory-based decisions}},
year = {2020},
eprint = {https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758%2Fs13423-020-01794-9},
customb = {https://osf.io/kbyas/}
}
@article{PaapeEtAl2017,
journal = {Glossa: {A} journal of general linguistics. },
title = {Does antecedent complexity affect ellipsis processing? An empirical investigation},
year ={2017},
Volume=2,
number=1,
pages={71},
doi = {10.5334/gjgl.290},
eprint ={https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/abstract/10.5334/gjgl.290/},
author = {Dario Paape and Bruno Nicenboim and Shravan Vasishth}
}
@article{NicenboimEtAl2016When,
issn = {1664-1078},
doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00280 },
eprint = {http://www.frontiersin.org/language_sciences/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00280/abstract},
number = {280},
year = {2016},
volume = {7},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology },
title = {When high-capacity readers slow down and low-capacity readers speed up: {Working} memory and locality effects},
customb = {https://github.com/bnicenboim/papers/tree/master/NicenboimEtAl2016.%20When%20High-Capacity%20Readers%20Slow%20Down%20and%20Low-Capacity%20Readers%20Speed%20Up:%20Working%20Memory%20and%20Locality%20Effects},
author = {Bruno Nicenboim and Pavel Logačev and Carolina Gattei and Shravan Vasishth}
}
@ARTICLE{NicenboimVasishth2016llcII,
author = {Bruno Nicenboim and Shravan Vasishth},
title = "Statistical methods for linguistic research: {Foundational} Ideas - {Part} {II}",
journal = {Language and Linguistics Compass},
eprint = {https://arxiv.org/abs/1602.00245},
pages = {591--613},
doi = {10.1111/lnc3.12207},
url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12207},
issn = {1749-818X},
number = {11},
volume = {10},
keywords = "rstanarm; tutorial",
year = "2016"
}
@article{NicenboimEtAl2018Number,
Title = {Exploratory and confirmatory analyses in sentence processing: {A} case study of number interference in {German}},
Author = { Bruno Nicenboim and Shravan Vasishth and Felix Engelmann and Katja Suckow},
journal ={Cognitive Science},
doi={10.1111/cogs.12589},
volume = {42},
number = {S4},
pages = {1075--1100} ,
year = {2018},
eprint ={https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/u2kqg/},
customb = {https://osf.io/4dg9b/}
}
@article{NicenboimEtAl2017EDAPS,
Title = {Using meta-analysis for evidence synthesis: {The} case of
incomplete neutralization in {German}},
Author = { Bruno Nicenboim and Timo B. Roettger and Shravan Vasishth },
Url = { },
doi = {10.1016/j.wocn.2018.06.001},
year = 2018,
eprint = {https://psyarxiv.com/p5a4z/},
customb = {https://osf.io/g5ndw/},
volume="70",
pages= "39--55",
journal= {Journal of Phonetics}
}
@article{VasishthEtAl2017EDAPS,
Title = {Bayesian data analysis in the phonetic sciences: {A} tutorial introduction},
Author = {Shravan Vasishth and Bruno Nicenboim and Mary E. Beckman and Fangfang Li and Eunjong Kong},
doi = {10.1016/j.wocn.2018.07.008},
eprint = {https://osf.io/5pj49/},
customb = {https://osf.io/g4zpv/},
volume="71",
pages= "147--161",
year = {2018},
journal= {Journal of Phonetics}
}
@unpublished{Vasishth_Nicenboim_Chopin_Ryder_2017,
title={Bayesian hierarchical finite mixture models of reading times: {A} case study},
year = {2017},
url={osf.io/fwx3s},
DOI={10.17605/OSF.IO/FWX3S},
notes={unpublished},
author={Vasishth, Shravan and Nicenboim, Bruno and Chopin, Nicolas and Ryder, Robin},
eprint = {https://osf.io/v5nps/}
}
@article{nicenboim_vasishth_rosler_2019,
title={Are words pre-activated probabilistically during sentence comprehension? {Evidence} from new data and a {Bayesian} random-effects meta-analysis using publicly available data},
eprint ={https://psyarxiv.com/2atrh/},
DOI={10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107427},
volume = {142},
author={Nicenboim, Bruno and Vasishth, Shravan and Rösler, Frank},
year={2020},
journal = "Neuropsychologia"
}
@article{lisson_etal_2020,
title={A computational evaluation of two models of retrieval processes in sentence processing in aphasia},
eprint={https://psyarxiv.com/r7dn5},
DOI={10.1111/cogs.12956},
author={Lisson, Paula and Pregla, Dorothea and Nicenboim, Bruno and Paape, Dario and van het Nederend, Mick L and Burchert, Frank and Stadie, Nicole and Caplan, David and Vasishth, Shravan},
journal = {Cognitive Science},
month = {apr},
publisher = {Wiley},
volume = {45},
number = {4},
year={2021}
}
@article{NicenboimVasishth2017Models,
Title = {Models of Retrieval in Sentence Comprehension: {A} computational evaluation using {Bayesian} hierarchical modeling},
Author = { Bruno Nicenboim and Shravan Vasishth },
volume = "99",
pages = "1 --34",
year = "2018",
issn = "0749-596X",
doi = "10.1016/j.jml.2017.08.004",
eprint = {https://arxiv.org/abs/1612.04174},
journal ={Journal of Memory and Language},
url = {https://arxiv.org/abs/1612.04174},
customb ={https://osf.io/a5ymk}
}
@article{vasishth_nicenboim_engelmann_burchert_2019,
title={Computational models of retrieval processes in sentence processing},
eprint ={https://psyarxiv.com/e4jds/},
DOI={10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.003},
volume = "23",
number = "11",
pages = "968 - 982",
issn = "1364-6613",
journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
author={Vasishth, Shravan and Nicenboim, Bruno and Engelmann, Felix and Burchert, Frank},
year={2019}
}
@Inproceedings{Nicenboim2018StanCon,
Title = {The implementation of a model of choice: {The} (truncated) linear ballistic accumulator},
Author = {Nicenboim, Bruno},
Booktitle = {{StanCon}},
Year = {2018},
doi = {10.5281/zenodo.1465990},
Location = {Aalto University, Helsinki, Finland},
Month = {8},
eprint = {https://htmlpreview.github.io/?https://github.com/stan-dev/stancon_talks/blob/master/2018-helsinki/Contributed-Talks/nicenboim/LBA_stancon2018.html}
}
@article{haines2018outcome,
title={The outcome-representation learning model: A novel reinforcement learning model of the iowa gambling task},
author={Haines, Nathaniel and Vassileva, Jasmin and Ahn, Woo-Young},
journal={Cognitive science},
volume={42},
number={8},
pages={2534--2561},
year={2018},
publisher={Wiley Online Library},
customb ={https://github.com/CCS-Lab/IGT}
}
@article{hBayesDM,
title = {Revealing Neurocomputational Mechanisms of Reinforcement Learning and Decision-Making With the {hBayesDM} Package},
author = {Ahn, Woo-Young and Haines, Nathaniel and Zhang, Lei},
journal = {Computational Psychiatry},
year = {2017},
volume = {1},
pages = {24--57},
publisher = {MIT Press},
url = {doi:10.1162/CPSY_a_00002},
customb ={https://github.com/CCS-Lab/hBayesDM}
}
@article{hainesanxiety,
title={Anxiety Modulates Preference for Immediate Rewards among Trait-Impulsive Individuals: A Hierarchical Bayesian Analysis},
author={Haines, Nathaniel and Beauchaine, Theodore P and Galdo, Matthew and Rogers, Andrew H and Hahn, Hunter and Pitt, Mark A and Myung, Jay I and Turner, Brandon M and Ahn, Woo-Young},
year = 2020,
journal = {Clinical Psychological Science},
customb ={https://github.com/CCS-Lab/Haines_2020_CPS}
}
@article{10.1167/16.2.4,
author = {Wallis, Thomas S. A. and Bethge, Matthias and Wichmann, Felix A.},
title = "{Testing models of peripheral encoding using metamerism in an oddity paradigm}",
journal = {Journal of Vision},
volume = {16},
number = {2},
pages = {4-4},
year = {2016},
month = {03},
abstract = "{ Most of the visual field is peripheral, and the periphery encodes visual input with less fidelity compared to the fovea. What information is encoded, and what is lost in the visual periphery? A systematic way to answer this question is to determine how sensitive the visual system is to different kinds of lossy image changes compared to the unmodified natural scene. If modified images are indiscriminable from the original scene, then the information discarded by the modification is not important for perception under the experimental conditions used. We measured the detectability of modifications of natural image structure using a temporal three-alternative oddity task, in which observers compared modified images to original natural scenes. We consider two lossy image transformations, Gaussian blur and Portilla and Simoncelli texture synthesis. Although our paradigm demonstrates metamerism (physically different images that appear the same) under some conditions, in general we find that humans can be capable of impressive sensitivity to deviations from natural appearance. The representations we examine here do not preserve all the information necessary to match the appearance of natural scenes in the periphery. }",
issn = {1534-7362},
doi = {10.1167/16.2.4},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1167/16.2.4},
eprint = {https://arvojournals.org/arvo/content\_public/journal/jov/934904/i1534-7362-16-2-4.pdf},
customb = {https://t.co/ggvYvnK4C1?amp=1}
}
@article {10.7554/eLife.42512,
article_type = {journal},
title = {Image content is more important than Bouma’s Law for scene metamers},
author = {Wallis, Thomas SA and Funke, Christina M and Ecker, Alexander S and Gatys, Leon A and Wichmann, Felix A and Bethge, Matthias},
editor = {Behrens, Timothy E and Herzog, Michael and Herzog, Michael and Caas, John},
volume = 8,
year = 2019,
month = {apr},
pub_date = {2019-04-30},
pages = {e42512},
citation = {eLife 2019;8:e42512},
doi = {10.7554/eLife.42512},
url = {https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.42512},
abstract = {We subjectively perceive our visual field with high fidelity, yet peripheral distortions can go unnoticed and peripheral objects can be difficult to identify (crowding). Prior work showed that humans could not discriminate images synthesised to match the responses of a mid-level ventral visual stream model when information was averaged in receptive fields with a scaling of about half their retinal eccentricity. This result implicated ventral visual area V2, approximated ‘Bouma’s Law’ of crowding, and has subsequently been interpreted as a link between crowding zones, receptive field scaling, and our perceptual experience. However, this experiment never assessed natural images. We find that humans can easily discriminate real and model-generated images at V2 scaling, requiring scales at least as small as V1 receptive fields to generate metamers. We speculate that explaining why scenes look as they do may require incorporating segmentation and global organisational constraints in addition to local pooling.},
keywords = {visual perception, scene appearance, texture perception, crowding},
journal = {eLife},
issn = {2050-084X},
publisher = {eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd},
customb ={https://zenodo.org/record/1475111}
}
@article{10.1167/17.12.5,
author = {Wallis, Thomas S. A. and Funke, Christina M. and Ecker, Alexander S. and Gatys, Leon A. and Wichmann, Felix A. and Bethge, Matthias},
title = "{A parametric texture model based on deep convolutional features closely matches texture appearance for humans}",
journal = {Journal of Vision},
volume = {17},
number = {12},
pages = {5-5},
year = {2017},
month = {10},
abstract = "{ Our visual environment is full of texture—“stuff” like cloth, bark, or gravel as distinct from “things” like dresses, trees, or paths—and humans are adept at perceiving subtle variations in material properties. To investigate image features important for texture perception, we psychophysically compare a recent parametric model of texture appearance (convolutional neural network [CNN] model) that uses the features encoded by a deep CNN (VGG-19) with two other models: the venerable Portilla and Simoncelli model and an extension of the CNN model in which the power spectrum is additionally matched. Observers discriminated model-generated textures from original natural textures in a spatial three-alternative oddity paradigm under two viewing conditions: when test patches were briefly presented to the near-periphery (“parafoveal”) and when observers were able to make eye movements to all three patches (“inspection”). Under parafoveal viewing, observers were unable to discriminate 10 of 12 original images from CNN model images, and remarkably, the simpler Portilla and Simoncelli model performed slightly better than the CNN model (11 textures). Under foveal inspection, matching CNN features captured appearance substantially better than the Portilla and Simoncelli model (nine compared to four textures), and including the power spectrum improved appearance matching for two of the three remaining textures. None of the models we test here could produce indiscriminable images for one of the 12 textures under the inspection condition. While deep CNN (VGG-19) features can often be used to synthesize textures that humans cannot discriminate from natural textures, there is currently no uniformly best model for all textures and viewing conditions. }",
issn = {1534-7362},
doi = {10.1167/17.12.5},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1167/17.12.5},
eprint = {https://arvojournals.org/arvo/content\_public/journal/jov/936521/i1534-7362-17-12-5.pdf},
customb = {https://zenodo.org/record/836726}
}
@misc{leuker_samartzidis_hertwig_2020,
title={What makes a market transaction morally repugnant?},
url={https://psyarxiv.com/dgz4s},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/dgz4s},
publisher={PsyArXiv},
author={Leuker, Christina and Samartzidis, Lasare and Hertwig, Ralph},
year={2020},
month={Apr}
}
@misc{greene_rhodes_2020,
title={A tutorial on Cognitive Modeling for Cognitive Aging Researchers},
url={https://psyarxiv.com/qsnea},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/qsnea},
publisher={PsyArXiv},
author={Greene, Nathaniel R and Rhodes, Stephen},
year={2020},
month={Sep}
}
@misc{tylen_fusaroli_smith_arnoldi_2020,
title={The social route to abstraction: interaction and diversity enhance rule-formation and transfer in a categorization task},
url={https://psyarxiv.com/qs253},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/qs253},
publisher={PsyArXiv},
author={Tylén, Kristian and Fusaroli, Riccardo and Smith, Pernille and Arnoldi, Jakob},
year={2020},
month={Aug}
}
@misc{ishkhanyan_fusaroli_hojen_tylen_johansson_christiansen_2020,
title={Cross-linguistic differences in categorical perception: {Comparison} of Danish and Norwegian},
url={https://psyarxiv.com/jpbtw},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/jpbtw},
publisher={PsyArXiv},
author={Ishkhanyan, Byurakn and Fusaroli, Riccardo and Højen, Anders and Tylén, Kristian and Johansson, Christer and Christiansen, Morten H},
year={2020},
month={Aug}
}
@article{Weed,
author = {Ethan Weed and Riccardo Fusaroli },
title = {Acoustic Measures of Prosody in Right-Hemisphere Damage: {A} Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis},
journal = {Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research},
volume = {63},
number = {6},
pages = {1762-1775},
year = {2020},
doi = {10.1044/2020\_JSLHR-19-00241},
eprint = {https://pubs.asha.org/doi/pdf/10.1044/2020\_JSLHR-19-00241}
}
@article {Tylen4578,
author = {Tyl{\'e}n, Kristian and Fusaroli, Riccardo and Rojo, Sergio and Heimann, Katrin and Fay, Nicolas and Johannsen, Niels N. and Riede, Felix and Lombard, Marlize},
title = {The evolution of early symbolic behavior in Homo sapiens},
volume = {117},
number = {9},
pages = {4578--4584},
year = {2020},
doi = {10.1073/pnas.1910880117},
publisher = {National Academy of Sciences},
abstract = {Early symbolic behavior of Homo sapiens is challenging to address yet arguably fundamental to the success of our species. We used ancient engravings from the South African Blombos Cave and Diepkloof Rock Shelter in a number of controlled cognitive experiments to qualify discussions about the evolution of early symbolic traditions. We found that the engravings evolved over a period of 30,000 y to become more effective {\textquotedblleft}tools for the mind,{\textquotedblright} that is, more salient to the human eye, increasingly expressive of human intent and identity, and easier to reproduce from memory. Our experiments suggest that the engravings served as decorations and expressions of socially transmitted cultural traditions, while we found no clear evidence that they served as denotational symbolic signs.How did human symbolic behavior evolve? Dating up to about 100,000 y ago, the engraved ochre and ostrich eggshell fragments from the South African Blombos Cave and Diepkloof Rock Shelter provide a unique window into presumed early symbolic traditions of Homo sapiens and how they evolved over a period of more than 30,000 y. Using the engravings as stimuli, we report five experiments which suggest that the engravings evolved adaptively, becoming better-suited for human perception and cognition. More specifically, they became more salient, memorable, reproducible, and expressive of style and human intent. However, they did not become more discriminable over time between or within the two archeological sites. Our observations provide support for an account of the Blombos and Diepkloof engravings as decorations and as socially transmitted cultural traditions. By contrast, there was no clear indication that they served as denotational symbolic signs. Our findings have broad implications for our understanding of early symbolic communication and cognition in H. sapiens.},
issn = {0027-8424},
URL = {https://www.pnas.org/content/117/9/4578},
eprint = {https://www.pnas.org/content/117/9/4578.full.pdf},
journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences}
}
@article{nolle2020language,
title={Language as shaped by the environment: linguistic construal in a collaborative spatial task},
author={N{\"o}lle, Jonas and Fusaroli, Riccardo and Mills, Gregory J and Tyl{\'e}n, Kristian},
journal={Palgrave Communications},
volume={6},
number={1},
pages={1--10},
year={2020},
publisher={Palgrave}
}
@article{PAROLA202024,
title = "Voice patterns in schizophrenia: A systematic review and Bayesian meta-analysis",
journal = "Schizophrenia Research",
volume = "216",
pages = "24 - 40",
year = "2020",
issn = "0920-9964",
doi = "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.11.031",
url = "http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0920996419305389",
author = "Alberto Parola and Arndis Simonsen and Vibeke Bliksted and Riccardo Fusaroli",
keywords = "Acoustic analysis, Social communication, Machine learning, Biomarker, Negative symptoms, Speech signal",
abstract = "Voice atypicalities have been a characteristic feature of schizophrenia since its first definitions. They are often associated with core negative symptoms such as flat affect and alogia, and with the social impairments seen in the disorder. This suggests that voice atypicalities may represent a marker of clinical features and social functioning in schizophrenia. We systematically reviewed and meta-analyzed the evidence for distinctive acoustic patterns in schizophrenia, as well as their relation to clinical features. We identified 46 articles, including 55 studies with a total of 1254 patients with schizophrenia and 699 healthy controls. Summary effect sizes (Hedges'g and Pearson's r) estimates were calculated using multilevel Bayesian modeling. We identified weak atypicalities in pitch variability (g = −0.55) related to flat affect, and stronger atypicalities in proportion of spoken time, speech rate, and pauses (g's between −0.75 and −1.89) related to alogia and flat affect. However, the effects were mostly modest (with the important exception of pause duration) compared to perceptual and clinical judgments, and characterized by large heterogeneity between studies. Moderator analyses revealed that tasks with a more demanding cognitive and social component showed larger effects both in contrasting patients and controls and in assessing symptomatology. In conclusion, studies of acoustic patterns are a promising but, yet unsystematic avenue for establishing markers of schizophrenia. We outline recommendations towards more cumulative, open, and theory-driven research."
}
@inproceedings{dideriksen2019contextualizing,
title={Contextualizing conversational strategies: {Backchannel}, repair and linguistic alignment in spontaneous and task-oriented conversations},
author={Dideriksen, Christina and Fusaroli, Riccardo and Tyl{\'e}n, Kristian and Dingemanse, Mark and Christiansen, Morten H},
booktitle={CogSci'19},
pages={261--267},
year={2019},
organization={Cognitive Science Society}
}
%% Shravan's file:
@article{paape2020quadruplex,
title = {Quadruplex negatio invertit? {T}he on-line processing of depth charge sentences},
author = {Paape, Dario and Vasishth, Shravan and von der Malsburg, Titus},
year = {2020},
journal = {Journal of Semantics},
note = {Accepted},
pdf = {https://psyarxiv.com/uw64a/},
customb = {https://osf.io/rb748/},
publisher = {Oxford University Press}
}
@article{BuerkiEtAl2020,
title = {What did we learn from forty years of research on semantic interference? {A Bayesian} meta-analysis},
author = {Audrey B{\"u}rki and Shereen Elbuy and Sylvain Madec and Shravan Vasishth},
year = {2020},
doi = {10.1016/j.jml.2020.104125},
customb = {https://osf.io/k6f4c/},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language}
}
@article{SchadEtAlWorkflow,
author = {Daniel J. Schad and Michael Betancourt and Shravan Vasishth},
title = {Towards a principled {B}ayesian workflow: {A} tutorial for cognitive science},
year = {2020},
pdf = {https://arxiv.org/abs/1904.12765},
customb = {https://osf.io/b2vx9/},
journal = {Psychological Methods},
note = {In Press}
}
@article{ALV2019,
author = {Avetisyan, Serine and Lago, Sol and Vasishth, Shravan},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
customb = {https://osf.io/ye98q/},
title = {Does case marking affect agreement attraction in comprehension?},
year = {2020},
volume = {112},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104087}
}
@article{JaegerMertzenVanDykeVasishth2019,
author = {J\"ager, Lena A. and Mertzen, Daniela and Van Dyke, Julie A. and Vasishth, Shravan},
title = {Interference patterns in subject-verb agreement and reflexives revisited: {A} large-sample study},
year = {2020},
volume = {111},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
customb = {https://osf.io/reavs/},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104063}
}
@article{VasishthEtAlTiCS2019,
title = {Computational models of retrieval processes in sentence processing},
author = {Shravan Vasishth and Bruno Nicenboim and Felix Engelmann and Frank Burchert},
journal = {Trends in Cognitive Sciences},
year = {2019},
volume = {23},
optissue = {11},
pages = {968--982},
pdf = {https://psyarxiv.com/e4jds},
customb = {https://osf.io/qtvxj/},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.09.003}
}
@article{paapehemforthvasishth2018,
title = {Processing of ellipsis with garden-path antecedents in {French and German}: {E}vidence from eye tracking},
author = {Dario Paape and Barbara Hemforth and Shravan Vasishth},
journal = {PLoS ONE},
year = {2018},
url = {http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0198620}
}
@article{VasishthMertzenJaegerGelman2018,
author = {Vasishth, Shravan and Mertzen, Daniela and J\"ager, Lena A. and Gelman, Andrew},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
url = {https://osf.io/eyphj/},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.07.004},
title = {The statistical significance filter leads to overoptimistic expectations of replicability},
year = {2018},
volume = {103},
pages = {151-175}
}
@article{JaegerEngelmannVasishth2017,
author = {J{\"a}ger, Lena A. and Engelmann, Felix and Vasishth, Shravan},
title = {Similarity-based interference in sentence comprehension: {Literature review and Bayesian meta-analysis}},
pdf = {http://www.ling.uni-potsdam.de/~vasishth/pdfs/JaegerEngelmannVasishthJML2017.pdf},
abstract = {We report a comprehensive review of the published reading studies on retrieval interference in reflexive-/reciprocal-antecedent and subject-verb dependencies. We also provide a quantitative random-effects meta-analysis of self-paced and eyetracking reading studies. We show that the empirical evidence is only partly consistent with cue-based retrieval as implemented in the ACT-R-based model of sentence processing by Lewis \& Vasishth 2005 (LV05) and that there are important differences between the reviewed dependency types. In non-agreement subject-verb dependencies, there is evidence for inhibitory interference in configurations where the correct dependent fully matches the retrieval cues. This is consistent with the LV05 cue-based retrieval account. By contrast, in subject-verb agreement as well as in reflexive-/reciprocal-antecedent dependencies, no evidence for interference is found in configurations with a fully cue-matching subject. In configurations with only a partially cue-matching subject or antecedent, the meta-analysis revealed facilitatory interference in subject-verb agreement and inhibitory interference in reflexives/reciprocals. The former is consistent with the LV05 account, but the latter is not. Moreover, the meta-analysis revealed that (i) interference type (proactive versus retroactive) leads to different effects in the reviewed dependency types; and (ii) the prominence of the distractor has an important impact on the interference effect.
In sum, the meta-analysis suggests that the LV05 needs important modifications to account for (i) the unexplained interference patterns and (ii) the differences between the dependency types. More generally, the meta-analysis provides a quantitative empirical basis for comparing the predictions of competing accounts of retrieval processes in sentence comprehension.},
year = {2017},
volume = {94},
pages = {316-339},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
customb = {https://github.com/vasishth/MetaAnalysisJaegerEngelmannVasishth2017},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.004}
}
@article{SafaviEtAlFrontiers2016,
author = {Molood Sadat Safavi and Samar Husain and Shravan Vasishth},
journal = {Frontiers in Psychology},
title = {Dependency resolution difficulty increases with distance in {P}ersian separable complex predicates: Implications for expectation and memory-based accounts},
optnote = {Special Issue on Encoding and Navigating Linguistic Representations in Memory},
pdf = {http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00403/full},
customb = {http://www.ling.uni-potsdam.de/~vasishth/code/SafaviEtAl2016DataCode.zip},
year = {2016},
volume = {7},
issue = {403},
optdoi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00403},
abstract = {Delaying the appearance of a verb in a noun-verb dependency tends to increase processing difficulty at the verb; one explanation for this locality effect is decay and/or interference of the noun in working memory. Surprisal, an expectation-based account, predicts that delaying the appearance of a verb either renders it no more predictable or more predictable, leading respectively to a prediction of no effect of distance or a facilitation. Recently, Husain et al (2014) suggested that when the exact identity of the upcoming verb is predictable (strong predictability), increasing argument-verb distance leads to facilitation effects, which is consistent with surprisal; but when the exact identity of the upcoming verb is not predictable (weak predictability), locality effects are seen. We investigated Husain et al.'s proposal using Persian complex predicates (CPs), which consist of a non-verbal element---a noun in the current study---and a verb. In CPs, once the noun has been read, the exact identity of the verb is highly predictable (strong predictability); this was confirmed using a sentence completion study. In two self-paced reading (SPR) and two eye-tracking (ET) experiments, we delayed the appearance of the verb by interposing a relative clause (Expt. 1 and 3) or a long PP (Expt. 2 and 4).
We also included a simple Noun-Verb predicate configuration with the same distance manipulation; here, the exact identity of the verb was not predictable (weak predictability). Thus, the design crossed Predictability Strength and Distance. We found that, consistent with surprisal, the verb in the strong predictability conditions was read faster than in the weak predictability conditions. Furthermore, greater verb-argument distance led to slower reading times; strong predictability did not neutralize or attenuate the locality effects. As regards the effect of distance on dependency resolution difficulty, these four experiments present evidence in favor of working memory accounts of argument-verb dependency resolution, and against the surprisal-based expectation account of Levy (2008). However, another expectation-based measure, entropy, which was computed using the offline sentence completion data, predicts reading times in Experiment 1. We suggest that forgetting due to memory overload leads to greater entropy at the verb.}
}
@article{SorensenVasishthTutorial,
title = {{B}ayesian linear mixed models using {S}tan: {A} tutorial for psychologists, linguists, and cognitive scientists},
year = {2016},
author = {Tanner Sorensen and Sven Hohenstein and Shravan Vasishth},
journal = {Quantitative Methods for Psychology},
pdf = {http://www.tqmp.org/RegularArticles/vol12-3/p175/p175.pdf},
url = {http://www.ling.uni-potsdam.de/~vasishth/statistics/BayesLMMs.html},
abstract = {With the arrival of the R packages \texttt{nlme} and \texttt{lme4}, linear mixed models (LMMs) have come to be widely used in experimentally-driven areas like psychology, linguistics, and cognitive science. This tutorial provides a practical introduction to fitting LMMs in a Bayesian framework using the probabilistic programming language Stan. We choose Stan (rather than WinBUGS or JAGS) because it provides an elegant and scalable framework for fitting models in most of the standard applications of LMMs. We ease the reader into fitting increasingly complex LMMs, using a two-condition repeated measures self-paced reading study.},
volume = {12},
number = {3},
pages = {175-200}
}
@article{LogacevVasishthQJEP2016,
title = {Understanding underspecification: {A} comparison of two computational implementations},
year = {2016},
journal = {Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology},
author = {Pavel Loga{\v c}ev and Shravan Vasishth},
volume = {69},
number = {5},
pages = {996-1012},
doi = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1134602},
customb = {https://github.com/plogacev/manuscript_LogacevVasishth_TQJEP_Underspecification/tree/master/Data_Swets_et_al},
pdf = {http://www.ling.uni-potsdam.de/~vasishth/pdfs/LogacevVasishthQJEP2016.pdf},
abstract = {Swets et al. (2008) present evidence that the so-called ambiguity advantage (Traxler et al., 1998), which has been explained in terms of the Unrestricted Race Model, can equally well be explained by assuming underspecification in ambiguous conditions driven by task-demands. Specifically, if comprehension questions require that ambiguities be resolved, the parser tends to make an attachment: when questions are about superficial aspects of the target sentence, readers tend to pursue an underspecification strategy. It is reasonable to assume that individual differences in strategy will play a significant role in the application of such strategies, so that studying average behavior may not be informative. In order to study the predictions of the good-enough processing theory, we implemented two versions of underspecification: the partial specification model (PSM), which is an implementation of the Swets et al. proposal, and a more parsimonious version, the non-specification model (NSM). We evaluate the relative fit of these two kinds of underspecification to Swets et al.’s data; as a baseline, we also fit three models that assume no underspecification. We find that a model without unspecification pro- vides a somewhat better fit than both underspecification models, while the NSM model provides a better fit than the PSM. We interpret the results as lack of unambiguous evidence in favor of underspecification; however, given that there is considerable existing evidence for good-enough processing in the literature, it is reasonable to assume that some underspecification might occur. Under this assumption, the results can be interpreted as tentative ev- idence for NSM over PSM. More generally, our work provides a method for choosing between models of real-time processes in sentence comprehension that make qualitative predictions about the relationship between several dependent variables. We believe that sentence processing research will greatly benefit from a wider use of such methods.}
}
@article{FrankEtAl2015,
author = {Stefan L. Frank and Thijs Trompenaars and Shravan Vasishth},
title = {Cross-linguistic differences in processing double-embedded relative clauses: {W}orking-memory constraints or language statistics?},
year = {2015},
pages = {554-578},
volume = {40},
doi = {10.1111/cogs.12247},
abstract = {An English double-embedded relative clause from which the middle verb is omitted can often be processed more easily than its grammatical counterpart, a phenomenon known as the grammaticality illusion. This effect has been found to be reversed in German, suggesting that the illusion is language specific rather than a consequence of universal working memory constraints. We present results from three self-paced reading experiments which show that Dutch native speakers also do not show the grammaticality illusion in Dutch, whereas both German and Dutch native speakers do show the illusion when reading English sentences. These findings provide evidence against working memory constraints as an explanation for the observed effect in English. We propose an alternative account based on the statistical patterns of the languages involved. In support of this alternative, a single recurrent neural network model that is trained on both Dutch and English sentences indeed predicts the cross-linguistic difference in grammaticality effect.},
journal = {Cognitive Science},
customb = {https://github.com/vasishth/StanJAGSexamples/tree/master/FrankEtAlCogSci2015},
pdf = {http://stefanfrank.info/pubs/GrammaticalityIllusion.pdf}
}
%%%
@article{burkner2019ordinal,
title={Ordinal regression models in psychology: A tutorial},
author={B{\"u}rkner, Paul-Christian and Vuorre, Matti},
journal={Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science},
volume={2},
number={1},
pages={77--101},
year={2019},
publisher={SAGE Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA}
}
@article{burkner2020modelling,
title={Modelling monotonic effects of ordinal predictors in Bayesian regression models},
author={B{\"u}rkner, Paul-Christian and Charpentier, Emmanuel},
journal={British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology},
year={2020},
publisher={Wiley Online Library}
}
%%
@article{Nalborczyk2019,
author = {Ladislas Nalborczyk and Cédric Batailler and Hélène Lœvenbruck and Anne Vilain and Paul-Christian B{\"u}rkner},
title = {An Introduction to Bayesian Multilevel Models Using brms: {A} Case Study of Gender Effects on Vowel Variability in Standard Indonesian},
journal = {Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research},
volume = {62},
number = {5},
pages = {1225-1242},
year = {2019},
doi = {10.1044/2018\_JSLHR-S-18-0006},
eprint = {https://pubs.asha.org/doi/pdf/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-S-18-0006}
}
@article{Molto,
author = {Lisa Molto and Ladislas Nalborczyk and Richard Palluel-Germain and Nicolas Morgado},
title ={Action Effects on Visual Perception of Distances: A Multilevel Bayesian Meta-Analysis},
journal = {Psychological Science},
volume = {31},
number = {5},
pages = {488-504},
year = {2020},
doi = {10.1177/0956797619900336},
note ={PMID: 32271656},
URL = {
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619900336
},
eprint = {
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619900336
}
,
abstract = { Previous studies have suggested that action constraints influence visual perception of distances. For instance, the greater the effort to cover a distance, the longer people perceive this distance to be. The present multilevel Bayesian meta-analysis (37 studies with 1,035 total participants) supported the existence of a small action-constraint effect on distance estimation, Hedges’s g = 0.29, 95\% credible interval = [0.16, 0.47]. This effect varied slightly according to the action-constraint category (effort, weight, tool use) but not according to participants’ motor intention. Some authors have argued that such effects reflect experimental demand biases rather than genuine perceptual effects. Our meta-analysis did not allow us to dismiss this possibility, but it also did not support it. We provide field-specific conventions for interpreting action-constraint effect sizes and the minimum sample sizes required to detect them with various levels of power. We encourage researchers to help us update this meta-analysis by directly uploading their published or unpublished data to our online repository (https://osf.io/bc3wn/). }
}
@misc{nalborczyk_banjac_celine_grandchamp_koster_perrone-bertolotti_loevenbruck_2020,
title={Dissociating facial electromyographic correlates of visual and verbal induced rumination},
url={psyarxiv.com/vfjn2},
DOI={10.31234/osf.io/vfjn2},
publisher={PsyArXiv},
author={Nalborczyk, Ladislas and Banjac, Sonja and Celine, Baeyens and Grandchamp, Romain and Koster, Ernst H W and Perrone-Bertolotti, Marcela and Loevenbruck, Hélène},
year={2020},
month={Mar}
}
@article{Nalborczyk_2020,
author = {Nalborczyk, Ladislas and Grandchamp, Romain and Koster, Ernst H. W. and Perrone-Bertolotti, Marcela and Lœvenbruck, Hélène},
journal = {PLOS ONE},
publisher = {Public Library of Science},
title = {Can we decode phonetic features in inner speech using surface electromyography?},
year = {2020},
month = {05},
volume = {15},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233282},
pages = {1-27},
abstract = {Although having a long history of scrutiny in experimental psychology, it is still controversial whether wilful inner speech (covert speech) production is accompanied by specific activity in speech muscles. We present the results of a preregistered experiment looking at the electromyographic correlates of both overt speech and inner speech production of two phonetic classes of nonwords. An automatic classification approach was undertaken to discriminate between two articulatory features contained in nonwords uttered in both overt and covert speech. Although this approach led to reasonable accuracy rates during overt speech production, it failed to discriminate inner speech phonetic content based on surface electromyography signals. However, exploratory analyses conducted at the individual level revealed that it seemed possible to distinguish between rounded and spread nonwords covertly produced, in two participants. We discuss these results in relation to the existing literature and suggest alternative ways of testing the engagement of the speech motor system during wilful inner speech production.},
number = {5},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0233282}
}
@article{NALBORCZYK_201753,
title = {Orofacial electromyographic correlates of induced verbal rumination},
journal = {Biological Psychology},
volume = {27},
pages = {53--63},
year = {2017},
issn = {0301-0511},
doi = {10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.04.013},
url = {http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0301051117300820},
author = {Ladislas Nalborczyk and Marcela Perrone-Bertolotti and Céline Baeyens and Romain Grandchamp and Mircea Polosan and Elsa Spinelli and Ernst H.W. Koster and Hélène Lœvenbruck}
}
@article{KentnerFranz2019,
title={No evidence for prosodic effects on the syntactic encoding of complement clauses in German},
author={Kentner, Gerrit and Franz, Isabelle},
journal={Glossa: a journal of general linguistics},
volume={4},
number={1},
pages={1--29},
year={2019},
customb={https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.565.s1}
}
@article{roettger_mapping_2019,
title = {Mapping prosody onto meaning–the case of information structure in {American} {English}},
volume = {34},
doi = {https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1587482},
number = {7},
journal = {Language, Cognition and Neuroscience},
author = {Roettger, Timo B and Mahrt, Tim and Cole, Jennifer},
year = {2019},
pages = {841--860},
customb={https://osf.io/4qxmh/}
}
@article{roettger_evidential_2019,
title = {Evidential {Strength} of {Intonational} {Cues} and {Rational} {Adaptation} to ({Un}-){Reliable} {Intonation}},
volume = {43},
issn = {1551-6709},
doi = {10.1111/cogs.12745},
number = {7},
journal = {Cognitive Science},
author = {Roettger, Timo B. and Franke, Michael},
year = {2019},
customb={https://osf.io/dnbuk/}
}
@article{roettger_listeners_2020,
title = {Listeners' adaptation to unreliable intonation is speaker-sensitive},
volume = {204},
issn = {0010-0277},
doi = {10/gg43nk},
journal = {Cognition},
author = {Roettger, Timo B. and Rimland, Kim},
month = nov,
year = {2020},
pages = {104372},
customb={https://osf.io/fdpg4/}
}
@article{soskuthy_when_2020,
title = {When the tune shapes morphology: {The} origins of vocatives},
volume = {5},
issn = {2058-458X},
doi = {10/gg9nc2},
number = {2},
urldate = {2020-09-03},
journal = {Journal of Language Evolution},
author = {Sóskuthy, Márton and Roettger, Timo B},
month = aug,
year = {2020},
pages = {140--155},
customb={https://osf.io/ejr8m/}
}
@article{roettger_toward_2019,
title = {Toward a replication culture: {Speech} production research in the classroom},
volume = {1},
issn = {2642-1828},
doi = {10.3765/pda.v1art4.13},
number = {4},
journal = {Phonological Data and Analysis},
author = {Roettger, Timo B. and Baer-Henney, Dinah},
month = nov,
year = {2019},
note = {Number: 4},
pages = {1--23},
customb = {https://osf.io/9kywf/}
}
@techreport{franke_bayesian_2019,
type = {preprint},
title = {Bayesian regression modeling (for factorial designs): {A} tutorial},
shorttitle = {Bayesian regression modeling (for factorial designs)},
url = {https://osf.io/cdxv3},
institution = {PsyArXiv},
author = {Franke, Michael and Roettger, Timo Benjamin},
month = jul,
year = {2019},
doi = {10.31234/osf.io/cdxv3},
customb = {https://github.com/michael-franke/bayes_mixed_regression_tutorial}
}
@article{Milne:2020aa,
author = {Milne, Andrew J. and Herff, Steffen A.},
journal = {Cognition},
pages = {104233},
title = {The perceptual relevance of balance, evenness, and entropy in musical rhythms},
volume = {203},
year = {2020},
eprint = {https://psyarxiv.com/5ue9a/},
customb = {https://osf.io/stnyg/}
}
@article{Smit:2019aa,
author = {Eline Adrianne Smit and Andrew J. Milne and Roger T. Dean and Gabrielle Weidemann},
journal = {PLOS ONE},
month = {06},
number = {6},
pages = {1-28},
title = {Perception of affect in unfamiliar musical chords},
volume = {14},
year = {2019},
eprint = {https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0218570}
}
@article{doi:10.1177/1029864920948575,
author = {Eline A. Smit and Andrew J. Milne and Roger T. Dean and Gabrielle Weidemann},
journal = {Musicae Scientiae},
pages = {1029864920948575},
title = {Making the unfamiliar familiar: {The} effect of exposure on ratings of unfamiliar musical chords},
year = {2020},
eprint = {https://www.researchgate.net/publication/343919664_Making_the_Unfamiliar_Familiar_The_Effect_of_Exposure_on_Ratings_of_Unfamiliar_Musical_Chords},
customb = {https://osf.io/6fg7b/}
}
@article{Stanford:2018aa,
author = {Sophia Stanford and Andrew J. Milne and Jennifer MacRitchie},
journal = {Applied Sciences},
number = {12},
pages = {2514},
title = {The effect of isomorphic pitch layouts on the transfer of musical learning},
volume = {8},
year = {2018},
eprint = {https://www.mdpi.com/376846},
customb = {https://osf.io/6vgph/}
}
@article{HerffENEURO.0413-19.2020,
author = {Herff, S. A. and Herff, C. and Milne, A. J. and Johnson, G. D. and Shih, J. J. and Krusienski, D. J.},
journal = {eNeuro},
title = {Prefrontal High Gamma in {ECoG} tags periodicity of musical rhythms in perception and imagination},
year = {2020},
eprint = {https://www.eneuro.org/content/7/4/ENEURO.0413-19.2020}
}
@article{abir2020getting,
title={Getting to the heart of it: Multi-method exploration of nonconscious prioritization processes},
author={Abir, Yaniv and Hassin, Ran R},
journal={Consciousness and Cognition},
volume={85},
pages={103005},
year={2020},
publisher={Elsevier},
customb = {https://doi.org/10.24433/CO.2463446.v1}
}
@article{Szewczyk_Power_of_good_2021_JEP_LMC,
title = {The power of “good”: {Can} adjectives rapidly decrease as well as increase the availability of the upcoming noun?},
shorttitle = {The power of “good”},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001091},
doi = {10.1037/xlm0001091},
journal = {Journal Of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, And Cognition},
author = {Szewczyk, Jakub M. and Mech, Emily N. and Federmeier, Kara D.},
year = {2021},
note = {10.1037/xlm0001091},
keywords = {N400, Neuroscience, sentence processing, Language, Cognitive Psychology, Social and Behavioral Sciences, incremental processing, noun phrases, updating},
customb = { https://osf.io/5rtn4/ },
eprint = { https://psyarxiv.com/ytaz3/ }
}